family types and the selection for nepotistic altruism

it finally clicked in my head while thinking about polygamy what the importance of family types — nuclear vs. extended, etc. — might be in the selection for altruistic behavioral traits, especially nepotistic altruism or clannishness. i should’ve thought through polygamy sooner instead of putting it off, but hey — procrastination is heritable, too, so in the words of h. solo, it’s not my fault! (~_^)

the logic of the mating patterns/inbreeding-outbreeding theory goes that, given the right set of circumstances (i.e. certain sorts of social environments), selection for nepotistic altruism/clannishness ought to go quicker or be amplified by inbreeding (close cousin marriage or uncle-niece marriage) simply because there will be more copies of any nepotistic altruism genes (alleles) that happen to arise floating around in kin groups. in other words, inbreeding should facilitate the selection for clannishness…if clannish behaviors are being selected for in a population.

the thing is, though: the individuals carrying certain versions (alleles) of nepotistic altruism genes need to direct their nepotistic behaviors towards other individuals carrying those same alleles, otherwise their actions will be for naught. (yeah. kin selection.) if they direct their nepotistic actions towards people who don’t share the same alleles, then the actions will be “wasted” and the behavioral traits won’t be selected for — or at least not very strongly — and they might fizzle out altogether.

let’s take an imaginary society as an example: say everyone in our pretend population always marries their first cousins. their father’s brother’s daughters (fbd) even, so that we get a lot of double-first cousin marriage. h*ck! let’s throw in some uncle-niece marriages on top of it all. the inbreeding coefficients in such a society would be very high, and if clannishness was being selected for in our highly inbred population, the selection ought to move pretty quickly.

but suppose we separated all the kids at birth from their biological families and set them out for adoption by unrelated individuals — people with whom they likely did not share the same nepotistic altruism alleles. think: the janissary system, only on a population-wide scale. if we did that, there should be virtually no selection for clannishness despite all the inbreeding since pretty much no one’s nepotistic behaviors would be directed towards other individuals with the same nepotistic altruism genes. in this case, kin selection would just not be happening.

such a society does not exist, and i don’t think ever has. but there are societies out there with certain family types — namely nuclear families (or even post-nuclear family societies!) — which ought to have a similar dampening effect on any selection for clannishness.

northwestern “core” europe has had very low cousin marriage rates since around the 800s-1000s, but it has also, thanks to manorialism, had nuclear families of one form or another (absolute or stem) since the early medieval period — nuclear families are recorded in some of the earliest manor property records in the first part of the ninth century from northeastern france [see mitterauer, pg. 59]. on the other hand, eastern europeans, like the russians and greeks, while they also seem to have avoided very close cousin marriage for several hundreds of years (which is not as long as northwestern europeans, but is quite a while), have tended to live in extended family groupings. you would think that nepotistic altruism could be selected for, or maintained more readily, in populations where extended family members lived together and interacted with one another on a more regular basis than in societies of nuclear family members where individuals interact more with non-kin. societies comprised of nuclear families are more like my hypothetical janissary society above where the altruism genes that might’ve been selected for via kin selection instead fade away in the wash.

we have to be careful, though, in identifying nuclear family societies. the irish of today, for instance, are typically said to be a nuclear family society, but the extended family does still interact A LOT (i can tell you that from first-hand experience). same holds true for the greeks and, i suspect, the southern italians. i would say that these populations have residential nuclear families, but not fully atomized nuclear families which have infrequent contact with extended family (think: the english). the early anglo-saxons in england were also characterized by residential nuclear families — the extended family (the kindred) was still very important in that society. the individuals in a residential nuclear family society probably do interact with non-family more than individuals in a society structured around extended families or clans, but less so than a true nuclear family society.

the thought for the day then?: family types can also affect the selection for clannishness/nepotistic altruism.

that is all! (^_^)

previously: polygamy, family types, and the selection for clannishness and “l’explication de l’idéologie”

(note: comments do not require an email. irish nuclear family.)

Advertisements

linkfest – 07/15/12

How Much Hard Evidence Do You Need?“[T]hree dopamine genes … examined, the dopamine transporter gene (DAT1), the dopamine D2 receptor gene (DRD2), and the dopamine D4 receptor gene (DRD4), had small but reliable effects on educational attainment, such that those with a higher ‘dopamine index’ tended to attain less education…. But what’s much more interesting is that they found significant differences in the prevalence of the alleles of theses genes between the White and the Black subjects, with Blacks having tending to have higher dopamine indices.” – from jayman (at his new location!).

Earliest Americans Arrived in Waves, DNA Study Finds – via steve sailer.

Spearheads and DNA Point to a Second Founding Society in North America“Clovis people, long known for their graceful, fluted projectile points, were not alone in the New World.” – original research article. see also: Paisley Caves yield 13,000-year old Western Stemmed points, more human DNA.

New Au. sediba fossils discovered in rock“Bones to be unearthed ‘live’ online, in a laboratory studio in real time.”

Fascinatin’ rhythm“[A] Neanderthal-derived segment that overlaps CLOCK, a key gene in regulating circadian rhythms. That segment has a frequency of 85% in Europeans.” – from greg cochran.

Pathological Altruism – jared taylor reviews Pathological Altruism.

Bad News for Big Brains“Humans and other creatures with large brains relative to their body size tend to have smaller guts and possibly fewer offspring. Scientists have debated for decades whether the two phenomena are related. Now a team of researchers says that they are—and that big brains do indeed make us smart.”

IQ and fertility cross-nationally – bad news from the inductivist.

The IQ’s Of Russian Politicians – anatoly guesstimates.

Intro to kinship and inbreeding coefficients“Fair warning: this is the most mathematical post I have ever done. If you hate math or are not good at it, don’t bother reading this post.” – from secular blood. also: On inbreeding coefficients and On kinship coefficients.

Women’s body movements are a potential cue to ovulation – something in the way she moves….

Natural Alzheimer’s Protection“Researchers identify a gene variant that reduces risk of Alzheimer’s disease.”

Q: Why Do We Wear Pants? A: Horses

bonus: Fruitflies evolve number sense

bonus bonus: Falling Dominoes – in china.

bonus bonus bonus: Feeling Snappy? Measuring Personality in Hermit Crabs

bonus bonus bonus bonus: Papua New Guinea charges 29 alleged cannibals

(note: comments do not require an email. lazy sunday.)

just what i wanted to know!

geneticists will tell you that the percentage of their genomes that two first-cousins will probably share is 6.25% (standard deviation ±2.4% — i’ve been wondering that, too!). but that’s in a randomly mating population. i’ve been wondering what happens in a population where cousin-marriage is the norm and has occurred generation after generation — like in saudi arabia or pakistan. what percentage of their genomes are those kinds of first-cousins likely to share?

well, now i have an answer. (it might not be the answer ’cause these researchers were looking only at families with congenital disorders, so … you know … ascertainment bias and all that.)

in “Quantification of Homozygosity in Consanguineous Individuals with Autosomal Recessive Disease,” woods, et. aaaaallll. (there’s a LOT of them), found that, in populations with regular, long-term cousin-marriage over many generations — in saudi arabians and pakistanis, as a matter of fact — first-cousins shared, on average, 11% of their genomes in common. that’s approaching twice as much as first-cousins in a randomly mating population.

“In conclusion, we found that the amount of homozygosity is greater than expected (11% observed vs. 6% expected) in individuals with autosomal recessive disease whose parents are first cousins and who come from communities that frequently practice consanguineous marriage. First-cousin offspring had as much homozygosity as would have been expected for double–first cousin offspring.”

so, first-cousin marriages in places like pakistan and saudi arabia (and afghanistan, etc., etc.) are, on average, more like double-first-cousin marriages in places where mating is more random, like large segments of the u.s., europe and the west in general. maybe.

here, once again, are mr. light-blue and ms. pink and their offspring, mr. dark-blue, to illustrate the differences for you (click on images for a LARGER view — should open in new tab/window):

mr. light-blue and ms. pink randomly mating:

here are the proud parents with their offspring, mr. dark-blue, who inherited the family fortune half of his genome from his father, and the other half from his mother:

here are mr. light-blue and ms. pink again, only this time they are first-cousins and share (prolly) 6.25% of their genomes in common (the bits that overlap):

here they are with blue, jr., who has still inherited half of his genome from each of his parents, but he’s got two identical (by descent) copies of some of his genome — he is less unique than mr. dark-blue above (see how he’s narrower?):

here are mr. light-blue and ms. pink one last time — they are first-cousins once again, but they come from a society where cousin marriage is the norm and are the descendants of a line of cousin marriages. the “overlap” of their genomes (11%) is larger than the first cousins above:

and here’s the final incarnation mr. dark-blue who is even less unique than mr. dark blue ii because he inherited even greater numbers of genes that are identical by descent from his parents:

(note: comments do not require an email. mr. pink.)

new and improved coefficients of inbreeding

so, just the other day i bored you to tears posted (with the invaluable help of the reluctant apostate!) some new and improved coefficients of relationship. (i haven’t quite finished calculating them all — one day, soon, i promise!)

why do i care about all these crazy coefficients of relationship? well, i don’t, really. what i wanted to get at were these other coefficient of inbreeding thingies, but i needed the relationship ones first, so … *sigh* … here we are at last.

without further ado, let me just post the new and improved coefficients of inbreeding that i have, and then i’ll explain afterwards what this is all about (see the previous post for the key to symbols; scroll down for the punchline):

F — s = 0.2460
s — F = 0.2460
F — d = 0.2541
d — F = 0.2500
M — s = 0.2500
s — M = 0.2541
M — d = 0.2500
d — M = 0.2500

B — B (both directions) = 0.2525
Z — Z (both directions) = 0.2564
B — Z = 0.2476
Z — B = 0.2436

PGF — s = 0.1255
s — PGF = 0.1255
PGF — d = 0.1205
d — PGF = 0.1186
MGF — s = 0.1270
s — MGF = 0.1270
MGF — d = 0.1270
d — MGF = 0.1250

PGM — s = 0.1186
s — PGM = 0.1205
PGM — d = 0.1314
d — PGM = 0.1314
MGM — s = 0.1250
s — MGM = 0.1270
MGM — d = 0.1250
d – MGM = 0.1250

FB — s = 0.1255
s — FB = 0.1255
FZ — s = 0.1186
s — FZ = 0.1205
MB — s = 0.1238
s — MB = 0.1238
MZ — s = 0.1282
s — MZ = 0.1303

FB — d = 0.1270
d — FB = 0.1250
FZ — d = 0.1250
d — FZ = 0.1250
MB — d = 0.1238
d — MB = 0.1218
MZ — d = 0.1282
d — MZ = 0.1282

s — FBS = 0.0652
s — FBD = 0.0603
s — MBS = 0.0603
s — MBD = 0.0635
s — FZS = 0.0603
s — FZD = 0.0603
s — MZS = 0.0651
s — MZD = 0.0651

d — FBS = 0.0593
d — FBD = 0.0657
d — MBS = 0.0593
d — MBD = 0.0625
d — FZS = 0.0625
d — FZD = 0.0625
d — MZS = 0.0641
d — MZD = 0.0641

so … eyes glazed over yet? (~_^)

what’s the point? the point is that, following steve sailer and parapundit and stanley kurtz’s leads regarding the effects of inbreeding on human societal behavior, i got to thinking that it’s not just inbreeding that matters but also the type of inbreeding. i think the type of inbreeding is important because we’re not all equally related to all of our relatives.

this very much includes our cousins who, in many societies, also become people’s husbands and wives. so, for instance, i don’t think it’s a coincidence that certain types of behaviors (mostly related to controlling reproduction) occur in societies where there is a high frequency of father’s brother’s daughter marriage.

now, when researchers look at the inbreeding rates in populations, they typically look at the coefficients of inbreeding (here’s an example — see the second-to-the-last column on the right). the usual coefficients of inbreeding look like this:

see first-cousins there? the inbreeding coefficient given is 0.0625. but, that’s not really correct since we are not related to all of our cousins in the same way. for instance, two male paternal cousins share a y-chromosome in common, whereas i don’t share a y-chromosome with any of my cousins since i don’t have one (a y-chromosome, that is — cousins i have a plenty!).

here are the actual inbreeding coefficients for cousins from the point-of-view of a guy (remember, these are probabilities — you might, in reality, be much more related to any given cousin, or not share any genes at all with another, although i think that’s pretty unlikely):

s — FBS = 0.0652
s — FBD = 0.0603
s — MBS = 0.0603
s — MBD = 0.0635
s — FZS = 0.0603
s — FZD = 0.0603
s — MZS = 0.0651
s — MZD = 0.0651

see? they’re not all the same. some are above the 0.0625 figure (which is probably some sort of average i guess) while some are below. so what?

well, if inbreeding does affect our behaviors (especially how we behave towards others), then inbreeding with someone with whom you are more related should accentuate whatever behaviors get affected by inbreeding in the first place. (btw, i think this effect would be stronger the more regular the inbreeding — like in saudi arabia where they’ve been marrying their cousins since before the arrival of islam.)

here’s an example — let’s look at a guy and which of his cousins he can marry. he can marry his father’s brother’s daughter [FBD], his father’s sister’s daughter [FZD], his mother’s brother’s daughter [MBD] or his mother’s sister’s daughter [MZD]. turns out that, from the point-of-view of the guy, he’s most related to his MZD. i would’ve thought that FBD marriage was the most inbred since the types of societies in which you find that sort of marriage seem to be the most clannish and tribal, but that’s not the case from the guy’s point of view. (i’ve included the numbers from the point-of-view of the female cousin|wife, as well. again, when a woman marries a cousin in an FBD arrangement, this is actually one of the least inbred cousin marriages she could enter.):

s — FBD = 0.0603 / d — FBS = 0.0593
s — FZD = 0.0603 / d — MBS = 0.0593
s — MBD = 0.0635 / d — FZS = 0.0625
s — MZD = 0.0651 / d — MZS = 0.0641

however, this is not the only way to consider inbreeding in a society. what happens when we start to look at the relationships of some of the other relatives in these different types of marriage systems? turns out that, from the point-of-view of the uncles or aunts in question, the father’s brother [FB] is the most related to his nephew (the groom):

FB — s = 0.1255
MB — s = 0.1238
MZ — s = 0.1282
FZ — s = 0.1186

that’s because, as i’ve mentioned before, a guy and his FB share a y-chromosome in common (see chart below; the FB = C on the chart) — and when a FB gets his daughter to marry his paternal nephew, he gets to “reunite” his y-chromosome, which his daughter does not carry, with part of his autosomal dna and part of his x-chromosome, which his daughter does carry:

we can even calculate the genetic relatedness of this grandfather (FB, or MGF from the point-of-view of the child) and his grandson. the grandson inherits, via his mother, 1/4 of the maternal grandfather’s autosomal dna plus 1/2 of his x-chromosome. from his father, the grandson inherits his maternal grandfather’s y-chromosome (which is virtually the same as his paternal grandfather’s y-chromosome!). so the calculation is (maths explained in this post):

1/4 autosome + 1/2 x-chromosome + y-chromosome =
(96.42% x 0.25) + (2.60% x 0.5) + 0.99% = 26.395% = 0.2640

without any inbreeding, the genetic relatedness between a MGF and a grandson is 0.2540.

if this is a pattern that holds true for other male relationships in societies where FBD marriage occurs, it may go a ways to explaining why those societies are so paternalistic, i.e. because the males are more related to one another than they are to the females and so, inclusive fitness-wise (if you can say that!), it’d be more in their genetic interests to help out their brothers and nephews and grandsons than their sisters and nieces and granddaughters.

i don’t know if this is true or not. i’m just speculating at this point. i want to run the numbers for different inbreeding scenarios to see what i come up with. might be something. might be nothing at all. stayed tuned….

previously: new and improved coefficients of relationship and all grandmas are not created equal and all cousins are not created equal and father’s brother’s daughter marriage and cousin marriage conundrum addendum

(note: comments do not require an email. did you want fries with that?)